Sunday, September 30, 2007

Boris: Wandering through Plato's cave

Boris is something of a chameleon in the drone doom community. Indeed, they might well object to any label being put on their creative output. While they do have songs in which single notes are drawn out until it seems they must snap and percussion is dispensed with as an unnecessary adulterant of pure tones and feedback, they also frequently display distinctly punk sensibilities, and are comfortable within the psychedelic rock idiom. Their vocals in particular display an intensity which seems rooted in political or personal concerns, rather than the existential terror or universal hatred which drip from the ragged edges of most black metal rasps. Being a drone doom aficionado myself, I think Boris reaches their peak when they veer towards the abstract and leave out the more quotidian vocals.

Their collaborations with Merzbow deserve particular mention. Merzbow seems to provide a textured but emotionally neutral backdrop against which Boris' pure tones can shine out like jewels. I've sometimes compared Ulver's Nattens Madrigal to being awoken late at night by a ringing telephone and picking up the receiver only to find God Himself on the line. The connection is poor and the line full of static, but it is abundantly clear that this is not the all forgiving God of the new testament, nor even the vengeful but rationally-minded God of the old testament. Rather, this is a being of wrath divorced from mortal notions of reason. Lovecraft's idiot flutist Azathoth blares into the line, barely constrained by the medium's bandpass filter, calling down an apocalypse which represents not moral judgment but the inevitable triumph of entropy. Just as Ulver conjures a deranged deity thrust into the modern world through the most banal tool of communication, Boris and Merzbow bring us into Plato's cave. The rough-hewn walls are solid, but devoid of intellectual or emotional presence. Against their mindless physicality dance pure ideas, freed from their earthly trappings by the stabilizing matrix of rock surrounding them. While the mental and the physical are inextricably wed, Boris and Merzbow draw the connection into a thin thread. The abstract and corporeal move independently, throwing each other into deeper contrast.

In their independent efforts, Boris provides this tension with the abstract through alternative routes. Mental and physical intertwine more tightly, but the result is a dance between yin and yang, rather than a uniform composite. Soaring notes reach out, only to be drawn under by the crash of cymbals and feedback. Their music often has an agitated energy which feels almost carnal, like the buzz of amphetamines, limbs vibrating and twitching of their own accord while the mind wanders elsewhere, only loosely coupled to the pumping pistons of the body. Other times, the music retreats into a contemplative fugue, acoustically fleshing out quiet corners of the world, speaking to lost moments spent alone, almost divorced from the self. In all cases, the result screams craftsmanship and quality.



Saturday, September 29, 2007

Computational horsepower: Natural vs artificial systems

An interesting exercise in dimensional analysis:

There are about 10^11 neurons in the brain, with about 10,000 synapses per neuron, yielding approximately 10^15 synapses total. Extracellular electrophysiology would have you believe that the average pyramidal (excitatory) neuron fires at about 10-20 Hz (inhibitory neurons fire even more rapidly), but there is a strong bias towards recording from more active neurons. It's hard to locate silent neurons with an extracellular electrode, since you can only determine that you are near a neuron when it fires. Intracellular electrodes may also be biased towards larger cells, since they are probably easier to spear (or clamp onto, depending upon technique), but almost all in vivo recordings are performed using extracellular electrodes. Arguments based upon metabolic rates suggest that the average firing rate is closer to 1 Hz, but I don't have the actual papers at my fingertips. I'll fish it out the reference if challenged. The traditional leaky-integrate-and-fire model of neural activity suggests that depolarization / shunting / hyperpolarization delivered to synapses throughout the dendritic tree and soma sum linearly in the soma (subject to a low-pas filter), and the neuron fires an action potential when its membrane potential exceeds some threshold. Polsky, Mel, and Schiller (2004), amongst other recent papers, implies that individual dendrites or dendritic compartments actually contain separate computational subunits, utilizing the nonlinear voltage-gated and NMDA channels to perform a thresholding operation similar to that normally ascribed to the axon hillock (where action potentials are initiated).

Let's be generous and assume that each thresholding operation roughly corresponds to a single floating point operation. We can also reasonably estimate that there are 100 such thresholding sites in the dendritic tree, and they perform thresholding operations (i.e., floating point operations) at the same rate that their inputs fire. The human brain then runs at approximately 10^11 neurons * 10^2 thresholding sites / neuron * 1 Hz = 10^13 flops. Bounding our estimate on the other side by assuming that each synapse performs a floating point operation each time it receives a spike and that the average neuron fires at 10 Hz, we find that the brain performs at most about 10^15 synapses * 10 Hz = 10^16 flops. The world's fastest supercomputers run at about 100 teraflops, or 10^14 flops, whereas desktop computers can achieve about 10^10 flops. Furthermore, Moore's law implies that computer performance should double approximately every 24 months. This suggests that computers are very close to the computational capacity of the brain.

Of course, this doesn't imply that we can simulate such a brain. The biophysics underlying these abstract computations is orders of magnitude more complex than the computations themselves. The blue brain project is currently struggling to simulate a single cortical column with a scant 10,000 neurons. To make efficient use of our available computational power to simulate the high-level activity of the brain, we would first need to know the basic algorithms the brain is computing.

Friday, September 28, 2007

Top search keywords

For your amusement, here are some of the search terms which led people to this blog:

amps that really go to 11
how do you purge after a binge?
martini & rossi vermouth global market share
math nightmares
what to do post binge
wolves in the thrown room (the band is wolves in the throne room; the homophone leads to a slightly different mental image)

Seek and ye shall find.

Oliver Sacks, on mountains of amphetamine, mescaline, and cannabis

Stolen without remorse from a Wired interview with Oliver Sacks about his new book on music and the brain:

Hume wondered whether one can imagine a color that one has never encountered. One day in 1964, I constructed a sort of pharmacological mountain, and at its peak, I said, "I want to see indigo, now!" As if thrown by a paintbrush, a huge, trembling drop of purest indigo appeared on the wall — the color of heaven. For months after that, I kept looking for that color. It was like the lost chord.

Then I went to a concert at the Metropolitan Museum of Art. In the first half, they played the Monteverdi Vespers, and I was transported. I felt a river of music 400 years long running from Monteverdi's mind into mine. Wandering around during the interval, I saw some lapis lazuli snuffboxes that were that same wonderful indigo, and I thought, "Good, the color exists in the external world." But in the second half I got restless, and when I saw the snuffboxes again, they were no longer indigo — they were blue, mauve, pink. I've never seen that color since.

It took a mountain of amphetamine, mescaline, and cannabis to launch me into that space. But Monteverdi did it too.

Sunday, September 23, 2007

Apophenia, take 2

After some additional consideration, I think my Apophenia post was unfair. Humans, and indeed mammals in general, are ridiculously good at detecting and utilizing correlations. But only particular types of correlations which they have been evolutionarily prepared to expect and process. Consider that darling of experimental neuroscience, the rat. Rats will learn to associate a tone with a foot-shock in only a few trials, if the tone is brief, co-terminates with the foot-shock, and if the tone predicts the shock with high reliability. If the tone is more than a few seconds long before the shock occurs, if there is a substantial gap between the tone and the shock, or if the tone occurs often without a shock, the rat will not learn the relationship. Similarly, rats can learn that water with a distinctive taste is correlated with nausea after only a single pairing, even if the nausea occurs hours after the water is consumed, but they cannot learn that lights and sounds are correlated with nausea.

You might now quite reasonably be thinking that learning to associate tones and shocks is not very impressive, and that I promised quality correlation detection. The tone-shock combinations are unnatural and thus akin to the correlations in the Apophenia post. Rats rarely encounter electrified grids hooked up to speakers in the wild. The ability to associate foods with illness is no mean feat, given the time spans involved (although it can go awry - if you've ever eaten a distinctive-tasting food while sick and later vomited, you likely found that you had lost your taste for that food. I didn't care much for lobster for most of my youth after my father brought home a special treat one evening when I had an ear infection. Cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy are often left staring down the business end of this phenomenon). But the brain (and the cortex in particular) really comes into its own when processing complex instantaneous sensory stimuli.

Consider the same rat which couldn't remember that it was going to receive a shock after hearing a bell because we nefariously inserted a five-second delay between the two stimuli. If you stick that rat in a big pool of opaque water (they use some sort of latex beads, I think) with a small platform hidden just below the surface of the water, the rat will swim around at random until stumbling upon the platform, at which point it will immediately climb up and out of the water (this is called a Morris water maze. It's generally used to test spatial memory. Note that while they're pretty good swimmers, rat's don't bathe recreationally). If you now pick the rat up, blindfold it, swing it around your head a few times to disorient it, and put it back in the pool at some random location, it will immediately swim back to the platform.

Amazing! After a single trial, the rat learned to associate the complex visual stimulus perceived at a particular location in the pool with the position of the platform. Even though this visual stimulus changes completely depending upon the direction in which the rat is facing (they don't have to approach the platform from the same direction each time). Even though the visual stimulus associated with other positions in the pool is virtually identical to that at the platform. Even though rats are not very visual animals (they rely primarily upon olfaction and tactile sensation (remember the whiskers!)). How the hell was the rat able to figure out that some subtle variation in the pattern of light falling on its retina indicates a nice dry spot to chill out, whereas almost identical patterns would leave it treading water until it drowned from exhaustion? (Of course, as the benevolent experimenter, you would rescue our friend the rat before it met its untimely demise in a kiddy-pool of milky water. Right? Right!?!?!)

Neither neuroscientists nor computer scientists have a convincing answer to this question. If you've ever tried to use voice-recognition software on a computer, you are familiar with how bad computers are at processing sensory input. The reason you've never even seen a computer vision recognition system is that they're even worse. State-of-the-art algorithms can recognize perhaps dozens of different categories of objects, but they are much less nuanced in their discriminations than a rat. For instance, most such systems are baffled by objection rotations and partial occlusion. The brain, in contrast, detects the necessary high-order correlations with such ease that you don't even realize how difficult the task is.

Saturday, September 22, 2007

Remember Clive Wearing

Every introductory course on neuroscience or psychology makes mention of H.M., a man who's medial temporal lobes (including the hippocampus and surrounding structures) were removed to treat drug-resistant epilepsy. After the operation, H.M. lost the ability to form new long-term memories, as well as most of his memory for the ten or so years before the surgery. (Ironically, H.M. has made a greater contribution to human knowledge than all but a handful of professional scientists, but he will never be able to appreciate the impact he's had.) H.M.'s case is canonical because it is the only instance where the connection between brain damage and subsequent amnesia is so clear. Indeed, no surgeon would have taken such drastic action had they known the effects, and no equivalent operation has been performed since (at least on humans). But H.M. is not alone in suffering from anterograde (can't form new memories) and graded retrograde (loss of memories of the recent past) amnesia. Korsakoff's amnesia is brought on by thiamine (vitamin B1) deficiency, primarily in alcoholics who derive a large percentage of their caloric intake from alcohol. The hippocampus can also be damaged by stroke, hypoxia, and infections.

One of the most acute known cases of amnesia is that of Clive Wearing (chronicled recently in this article by Oliver Sacks), whose temporal and frontal lobes were damaged by herpes encephalitis. Unlike H.M. and other amnesiacs, Mr. Wearing remembers nothing at all; his entire experience is restricted to the minute or two available through working memory (the short term memory which underlies active though processes, often believed to have a capacity of 7 +/- 2 "chunks"). Every time Mr. Wearing is distracted, he awakens to an entirely new and unfamiliar reality. For years, Mr. Wearing has kept a diary. Each entry contains the current time and a record of the profound realization that he is now, for the first time, alive and conscious. He then notices the previous entries. Pages of them. All making the same claim. All in his own familiar handwriting. All written by some unremembered stranger. He goes back, systematically crosses out these false entries, and underlines the current entry. The first true entry. He sets the diary down, glances out the window, and awakens for the first time.



Mr. Wearing has more to teach us than the dependency of memory formation and access on certain brain structures. His condition is not so different from our own. Consciousness is inextricably tied to the present moment. Our past and our future belong to other people. People who occupy the same body, in the same world, but who are tied together only by the hallucination of memory.



Apophenia

Humans are horrible at noticing unexpected connections between large number of variables. When I was living in LA, driving took on an added measure of excitement during a heavy rainfall. Not because of reduced visibility or the slickness of the road as months of impacted dust and oil were finally loosened and left to form a thin, low-viscosity film over the asphalt. Rain has those effects on roadways across the world. In New York or Boston, the dangers of driving in the rain are hammered into new motor vehicle operators before the first drop of precipitation taps their moving windshields. Southern Californian drivers, in contrast, are faced with rain so infrequently that the subject seems to be passed over in high school driver's ed. Many are slow to discover the relationship between rain and reduced traction on their own. Even the occasional skid while blasting through tight curves at 60 mph or faster doesn't seem to clue them in to a possible causal interaction. On the evening after a heavy rainfall, there is inevitably a car or two lying inoperable on the sides of the 110. The stories I heard about cars spinning 360 in the middle of the roadway were even more disturbing.

Another example: One summer in college, I was baffled by the seemingly unpredictable variations in my level of motivation when working in the afternoons. Some days I was hungry and tired and disengaged as the afternoon wore into the evening. Other days, I was consumed by my research and barely thought about dinner. I thought the difference might be due to some aspect of my diet or sleep pattern, but I couldn't find any consistent covariations. I even considered the possibility that I was going into ketosis (which of course is patently absurd). Only after the fact did I come to realize that the difference must have been due to the color of the coffee pot. I had always assumed that caffeine had relatively little effect on me and drank coffee because I liked the taste. So I didn't really pay attention to whether I poured my coffee from the brown (caffeinated) pot, or the orange (decaffeinated) pot. In retrospect, I was as naive as the LA drivers who refuse to ease up on the gas peddle during a downpour.

Which brings us to the present day. Up until last Tuesday, I was in a multi-week funk, feeling not just tired, but less conscious than usual. I was sleeping long and deeply, but it just wasn't doing me any good. Philosophers refer to entities that behave just like people but which aren't conscious as zombies. That's what I felt like, perhaps with the addition of a little homunculus sealed off behind one-way, sound-proof glass, allowed to watch the proceedings but unable to exert any control. Then on Tuesday, the funk magically lightened. I can't say I feel 100%, but it's certainly better. What brought on this descent into the voodoo nether-world? What precipitated my slow return? Here we are once again faced with a superflux of variables and no sign of a correlation. I'm sure there's a simple explanation, but I'm equally sure that I will never find it. My best guesses are:

a) After spending a week or two going all hard-core on the German learning, I gave up again. I can't remember the citation off the top of my head, but I'm pretty sure that REM sleep has been shown to be correlated with language learning. That is, immersion in a foreign language increases the amount of REM sleep, and the magnitude of this increase is correlated with the amount of learning. Perhaps my foolish attempts to learn the local language were saturating my REM sleep time, leaving me semi-sleep deprived despite eight hours in bed.

b) The gym finally reopened. It had been closed for the previous two weeks. For cleaning. In the US, the cleaning would have been done at night. Or one section of the facility would have been closed off at a time, leaving the rest functional and open. I can only assume that a phalanx of temporary employees was brought in, issued toothbrushes, and spent ten eight-hour days on their hands and knees, scouring every surface. Either that or they sealed off all the entrances and flooded the building with dilute hydrogen peroxide. When I was finally allowed back in Monday night, the gym was indeed clean. But it was clean before they shut down. Which is to say, I couldn't see that their extensive efforts made any difference. Regardless, I was finally able to lift heavy things and put them down again, after two weeks of just running in circles. This change in my exercise routines may have directly or indirectly affected my energy level.

In the end, I'm probably just an obsessive-compulsive hypochondriac, and it was all in my head to begin with. But the point remains that people are bad at detecting unexpected correlations.

Monday, September 17, 2007

Coffee machine: The legend continues

So I managed to get to the Migros on Saturday with a few minutes to spare before the 5pm closing time. I don't think my roommate bought my story that the coffee machine's great aunt was a phoenix, and that it was consumed in a burst of flame and born anew from the ashes. The slag of its predecessor in the sink might have tipped her off.

On Sunday, I set out to make my first cup of coffee with the gleaming new aluminum marvel. I even managed to read the instructions in German only to discover that, for the past year, I've been packing in the coffee grounds too tight and brewing the coffee over excessive heat. With my ground coffee loosely set in the filter and the flame turned down to medium, I awaited the black nectar that would soon gush from the top of the spout. Except that it didn't. Rather, it bubbled out of the side of the machine and pooled on the top of the oven. I did manage to extract enough juice to have my morning cup of coffee, but the residue in the bottom of the pot revealed a rift extending along perhaps a quarter of the bottom edge of the pot.

Made in Italy. Worthless. Apparently, Italian aluminum is very delicate and tears when subjected to too much force. Even when it is a couple of millimeters thick.

So I'm now on my third coffee machine in three days. Folk wisdom holds that this attempt should be charmed, but I'm not holding my breath.

Saturday, September 15, 2007

Swiss apartments don't have smoke detectors

One of the things you don't realize living in America is how new everything is. A country that is less than 250 years old, and which has been subject to constant rapid growth, necessarily has little in the way of old buildings. As a result, few current buildings have been grandfathered past modern safety regulations. As a case in point, I can't think of a single building I've visited in America which didn't have smoke detectors. In just about any kitchen in the country, were you to leave a two-part screw-together espresso machine on the burner after the coffee had finished sputtering out of the top, the discordant shriek of a smoke alarm would soon alert you to your folly.

In contrast, in Switzerland, were you to start a cup of espresso and retire to your room, the first signal to wake you from your obliviousness would be the scent of something burning slowly diffusing through the crack under the door from the billowing clouds of smoke out in the hallway. You might then rush into the hazy kitchen to find the top of the stove covered with a crust of long-since dried coffee, the knob on the top of the coffee machine melted into a Dali-like parody of itself, and the plastic handle simply gone. You would be forced to conclude that under the influence of extreme and prolonged heat, seemingly durable plastic handles can disintegrate outright.

Switzerland would then place you in an additional bind, as your hankering for coffee might have hit at around 4pm and all of the stores close for the weekend at 5pm, leaving you with 15 minutes to ride your skateboard (your bike having been in the repair shop for the past two weeks; quality work apparently takes a lot of time) to the store to buy a replacement. Mach schnell!!!

Sunday, September 9, 2007

All for naught

Lately, I've had a hankering for a martini. I already had a bottle of gin, so this weekend I went to the supermarket to purchase some olives and a bottle of vermouth. Dry vermouth. The olives were easy, as you would expect, and the alcohol counter had four different types of vermouth. I have generally assumed that vermouth comes in two different varieties: dry (for martinis) and sweet (for leaving on the store shelf). Swiss supermarkets apparently split the world along different lines. Their display distinguished between rot (red) and weiss (white) vermouth. Rather than plunge headlong into the unknown and wind up with something undrinkable, I confronted the salesperson, in German no less, and asked whether any of the bottles contained dry vermouth. After a minute or so of mutually semi-intelligible mumbling, she decided that she didn't know, and asked another clerk. Another minute of garbled German later, and a third clerk was paged to the alcohol counter. And then paged again after five minutes of standing around uselessly. After he managed to communicate his unfamiliarity with the distinction between sweet and dry vermouth, I just bought a bottle marked Martini & Rossi, white. I know that Martini & Rossi makes the canonical vermouth, and I've never put anything red in a martini, so by the process of elimination, this seemed like a reasonable choice. Little did I know that the Italian company labels their sweet vermouth "bianco," and that in a country where an order for a "martini" in a bar produces a glass full of vermouth, rather than a glass of gin over which a closed bottle of vermouth has been quickly passed, supermarkets don't even carry dry vermouth, at least of the Martini & Rossi brand. Why, I am forced to ask, do I waste any time trying to learn German when it proves to be of so little use in practice?

In other news, Terminator 2 in German sounds completely and totally wrong. Arnold's Austrian accent is well enough preserved, but carefully crafted pithy phrases like "I'll be back" just don't translate.

Saturday, September 8, 2007

Clashing subcultures

Last night, I went to a goth event at Dynamo, a "youth culture house." Dynamo has two spaces for holding music-based events: a large hall suitable for a few hundred people on the fifth floor, and what looks like a cave carved out of the basement, which can accommodate perhaps 75 people without risk of suffocation or trampling. The entire complex seems to be maintained by an ouroboros of angry teenagers and twenty-somethings, the youth at the head constantly consuming the jaded veterans. In the main basement room, the walls of rough-hewn stone curve up towards the ceiling like a large train tunnel accidentally lost in subterranean Zurich during the industrial revolution. Sophisticated modern club lights are bolted directly to the unfinished ceiling. They flash and twirl mindlessly, without regard to the tempo or character of the music. In the place of a fog machine, a box fan or two hum in the corners to reduce the risk of hypoxia. I hope that the smaller girls will serve as the canaries in this mine, but my manic dancing probably means that I will be the first to fall.

Dynamo has goth nights approximately twice per month, produced by various groups, and invariably confined to this basement. The building is set into the side of a steep river bank, so when walking to the dungeon, I pass by the upper floors. Last night, the walkways and stairs leading from the street down to the basement were covered with what looked like an international punk convention. In an affluent, mild-mannered place like Switzerland, you'd think that the youth would have relatively little to rebel against, but comfort and security often breed contempt. The Swiss equivalent of America's white middle-class suburban ganstas seems to be a carefully preened pseudo-punk, replete with faux-hawk, tight jeans, and Converse All-Stars. This forces the counter-culture to go even further. In 100 feet, I saw enough metal both wrapped around and pierced through the massed bodies to outfit a small hardware store. Dyed hair and mohawks were definitely the order of the day. Few articles of clothing had been spared intentional damage and reconstruction.

The grand irony of this brief saunter through punk never-never-land is that the punk aesthetic is not so different from goth culture. Both forms of music share common roots. And yet, there was an unmarked but blindingly obvious line dividing the punks from the goths. Even middle school cafeterias permit less rigidly demarcated social stratifications. When I left for the night at 3am, there were still clumps of punks milling around Dynamo. I imagine that their concert ended hours before, but since public transportation would not shake off its nightly torpor for another three hours, they were just smoking cigarettes while watching for the dawn. I saw only a single punk breach the line and wander into the basement, where it was warm and the beer was still flowing.

Friday, September 7, 2007

Every week is fashion week

I read the New York Time web site like a holy writ. Frequently, coverage of some fashion show or another will make its way to the front page of the web site. I'm a sucker for anorexics, so I generally take the bait and click the link. Of course, being the sort of person who reads the entirety of every blurb in a museum (even if it's an art museum (even if the text just list the artist, year of creation, and the ever-popular name "Untitled.")), I actually read the article that accompanies the pictures of expensively dressed waifs and wastrels. So far as I can tell, though, these articles are as free of content as the carefully coiffed heads of the models.

The world of fashion seems to be predicated on some notion of progress, where styles constantly change and evolve. Certainly, there is some basis for a claim of development in other visual art forms. The style of paintings exhibit clear changes from decade to decade, and while I am no art critic, I can appreciate some of the evolving theoretical ideas which underlie the changes in appearance. These runway shows, in contrast, feature the same basic design year after year. Shirts are still shirts. Pants are still pants. The commentators will note that wool is a common trend this year. Last year there was very little wool. But seven years ago everything was wool. Does this make wool new or innovative?

What's more, I cannot for the life of me figure out whether anything motivates the cut of the sleeve of a dress or the choice of lining for a coat other than pure aesthetic preference divorced from deeper metaphor or meaning. I mean, a designer presents a line of coats which neither keep the wearer warm nor allow them a full range of vision. Fantastic. But why? Is this commentary on the position of women in society? Or the relationship of the individual to the technological and cultural artifacts with which we surround ourselves? Because it looks really weird and doesn't really function as clothing in the conventional sense. Even if you could manage to walk around in a high fashion outfit without tripping or running into a wall, your progress would be impeded by the baffled masses gawking at the such alien tactics for covering your nakedness. If runway clothing is so impractical that it cannot be worn outside of a fashion show (hence the necessity of distinguishing "ready-to-wear" fashion from its more cumbersome brethren), then surely these aesthetic flourishes should serve some higher purpose. Please enlighten me.

Wednesday, September 5, 2007

Hot new antidepressant: LSD

On page 1218 of Kandel, Schwartz, and Jessell's canonical Principles of Neural Science (4th ed), a diagram describing the "Action of antidepressant and other drugs at seratonergic and noradrenergic synapses" contains the following annotation for the post-synaptic 5-HT receptor: (Antidepressant) "stimulation of 5-HT receptors as partial agonist (lysergic acid diethylamide)." And in the caption: "Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) acts as a partial agonist at postsynaptic serotonergic receptors in the central nervous system. A number of specific compounds are now candidates to act as receptor-blocking agents at various serotonergic synapses."

Jesus Christ! My understanding is that most psychedelic substances are agonists of the 5-HT2a receptor. In fact, I think I've read some papers which used 5-HT2a stimulation (in the rat aorta of all places) as a test of hallucinogenic activity. But I've never thought of these substances as anti-depressants, despite their common activity in the serotonin system. Of course, antidepressants (all of which increase the availability of serotonin in the synapse, at least initially) take weeks to have clinical effects despite their immediate impact on the serotonin system (well, at least SSRI's and the tricyclics have immediate effects), so it's unlikely that a substance that is washed from the body in a matter of hours would have an effect on depression due to its serotonergic activity.